Do you want to improve your writing? Try our new evaluation service and get detailed feedback.
Check Your Text it's free

what is the best way to make kids learn?

what is the best way to make kids learn? 7JNm3
When we look back on our own school days, our strongest memories are probably a mix of big occasions—field trips, plays, and sports days alongside more personal events tinged with strong emotion. Things that happened that were really funny or sad, or that made us feel excited, interested, exhilarated, or angry. We don’t tend to remember vividly, if at all, actually learning the substance of math or English or design technology. We might remember amusing anecdotes from lessons gone awry, or still bristle at past injustices — “but I wasn’t talking” — or have a vague impression of sitting in the science lab, with fleeting snippets of memories of this or that experiment. All of which leads to us making the entirely reasonable hypothesis that if we want students to remember what we teach them, then we need to make our lessons more like the spectacular one-off special events, or, at the very least, involve something specially selected because it’s exciting and possibly unusual. Memorable events, in this view, should form the template for creating memorable lessons. As reasonable as this seems, this is a myth. It is a myth because human memory works in two different ways, both equally valid but one of which is much better at enabling us to transfer what we have learnt to new contexts. This transfer is an essential prerequisite for creativity and critical thinking. The two forms of memory are known as episodic and semantic memory. Episodic memory is the memory of the ‘episodes’ of our life—our autobiographical memory. This takes no effort on our part, it simply happens. We don’t have to try consciously to remember what happened yesterday. Those memories just happen automatically. But there is a downside. Episodic memory is “easy come, easy go. ” If you try to remember what you had for lunch yesterday, you will probably remember. If you try to remember what you had for lunch a year ago today—unless that happened to be some very significant date and some particularly noteworthy lunch—you will have no idea. Semantic memory, on the other hand, involves much harder work. We have to expend effort to create semantic memories. This is the kind of memory we use when we consciously study something because we want to remember it. Unlike episodic memory, it does not just happen. The upside, however, is that the effort involved results in a long lasting memory. Have you ever been in a course where you have really enjoyed listening to the speaker, found the subject matter interesting and the presenter amusing and engaging. Yet when you try to explain to someone the next day what the course was about, all that is really left is a vague impression of your emotions during the day, tinged with the odd snippet of content? You know the course was really good yet can’t really explain what it was actually about beyond the most general of assertions. That’s because at that point your memories are mainly episodic and are already fading. This is particularly likely to have happened if you just listened to the speaker rather than making some notes, and if you didn’t have to do activities during the day that made you think hard about the content. But even if you did, unless you reread those notes some time later, or read the PowerPoint, or the blogs they mentioned, or plan a staff meeting to tell others about what you have learnt, unless you expend some effort in revisiting the message, however inspiring you found the message at the time, your memory of actual specifics will quickly fade away—leaving you with at best fond memories of an enjoyable and interesting day.
When we look back on our
own
school days, our strongest
memories
are
probably
a mix of
big
occasions—field trips, plays, and sports days alongside more personal
events
tinged with strong emotion. Things that
happened
that were
really
funny
or sad, or that made us feel excited, interested, exhilarated, or angry. We don’t tend to
remember
vividly
, if at all, actually learning the substance of math or English or design technology. We might
remember
amusing anecdotes from lessons gone awry, or
still
bristle at past injustices — “
but
I wasn’t talking” — or have a vague impression of sitting in the science lab, with fleeting snippets of
memories
of this or that experiment. All of which leads to us making the
entirely
reasonable hypothesis that if we want students to
remember
what we teach them, then we need to
make
our lessons more like the spectacular one-off special
events
, or, at the
very
least, involve something
specially
selected
because
it’s exciting and
possibly
unusual. Memorable
events
, in this view, should form the template for creating memorable lessons.

As reasonable as this seems, this is a myth. It is a myth
because
human
memory
works in two
different
ways, both
equally
valid
but
one of which is much better at enabling us to transfer what we have
learnt
to new contexts. This transfer is an essential prerequisite for creativity and critical thinking.

The two forms of
memory
are known
as
episodic
and semantic
memory
.
Episodic
memory
is the
memory
of the ‘episodes’ of our life—our autobiographical
memory
. This takes no
effort
on our part, it
simply
happens. We don’t
have to
try
consciously
to
remember
what
happened
yesterday. Those
memories
just
happen
automatically
.
But
there is a downside.
Episodic
memory
is “easy
come
, easy go. ” If you try to
remember
what you had for lunch yesterday, you will
probably
remember
. If you try to
remember
what you had for lunch a year ago
today
—unless that
happened
to be
some
very
significant date and
some
particularly
noteworthy lunch—you will have no
idea
.

Semantic
memory
,
on the other hand
, involves much harder work. We
have to
expend
effort
to create semantic
memories
. This is the kind of
memory
we
use
when we
consciously
study something
because
we want to
remember
it. Unlike
episodic
memory
, it does not
just
happen. The upside,
however
, is that the
effort
involved results in a
long lasting
memory.

Have you ever been in a course where you have
really
enjoyed listening to the speaker, found the
subject matter
interesting and the presenter amusing and engaging.
Yet
when you try to
explain
to someone the
next
day what the course was about, all
that is
really
left
is a vague impression of your emotions during the day, tinged with the odd snippet of content? You know the course was
really
good
yet
can’t
really
explain
what it was actually about beyond the most general of assertions. That’s
because
at that point your
memories
are
mainly
episodic
and are already fading. This is
particularly
likely to have
happened
if you
just
listened to the speaker
rather
than making
some
notes, and if you didn’t
have to
do activities during the day that made you
think
hard
about the content.
But
even if you did, unless you reread those notes
some
time later, or read the PowerPoint, or the blogs they mentioned, or plan a staff meeting to
tell
others about what you have
learnt
, unless you expend
some
effort
in revisiting the message,
however
inspiring you found the message at the time, your
memory
of actual specifics will
quickly
fade away—leaving you with at best fond
memories
of an enjoyable and interesting day.
What do you think?
  • This is funny writingFunny
  • I love this writingLove
  • This writing has blown my mindWow
  • It made me angryAngry
  • It made me sadSad

IELTS essay what is the best way to make kids learn?

Essay
  American English
5 paragraphs
614 words
5.5
Overall Band Score
Coherence and Cohesion: 5.5
  • Structure your answers in logical paragraphs
  • ?
    One main idea per paragraph
  • Include an introduction and conclusion
  • Support main points with an explanation and then an example
  • Use cohesive linking words accurately and appropriately
  • Vary your linking phrases using synonyms
Lexical Resource: 5.0
  • Try to vary your vocabulary using accurate synonyms
  • Use less common question specific words that accurately convey meaning
  • Check your work for spelling and word formation mistakes
Grammatical Range: 6.5
  • Use a variety of complex and simple sentences
  • Check your writing for errors
Task Achievement: 5.0
  • Answer all parts of the question
  • ?
    Present relevant ideas
  • Fully explain these ideas
  • Support ideas with relevant, specific examples
Labels Descriptions
  • ?
    Currently is not available
  • Meet the criteria
  • Doesn't meet the criteria
Recent posts