In the earliest years of life, language acquisition is considered one of the most remarkable milestones in children’s development and can be a basis for psychological and cognitive development (Varanda et al, 2019). Therefore, children who suffer from language disorders might experience a range of other successive developmental issues, which negatively affect their transition to adulthood. Unfortunately, language difficulties were reported to be amongst the most common developmental issues in early childhood which were calculated to be between 7 to 17 percent in prevalence (Law, Boyle, Harris, Harkness, & Nye, 1999; Reilly et al. , 2010; Cronin et al. , 2016). Generally, children who have difficulties in verbal communication, are kindergarten children around 5 to 6 years of age (Beitchman et al. , 1986; Tomblin et al. , 1997; Norbury et al. , 2016; Lindsay & Strand, 2016). In the following essay, the insightful understanding of developmental language disorders along with the recommended treatment and therapy will be discussed.
According to the National Institute on Deafness and other Communication Disorders (NIDCD) (2019), children who do not struggle with hearing or intellectual disabilities but whose developmental language skills are delayed due to their communication disorders can be identified as Specific Language Impairment (SLI). McDevitt and other researchers (2019) have asserted that infants normally start forming and saying their first few simple and recognizable words at about 12 months of age. Furthermore, many nativists believed that there is an innate language ability in the early ages which actively demonstrates that during a child’s early experience, he can naturally acquire the language despite no specific instruction is given (McDevitt et al. , 2019). Chomsky (1959) called this biologically in-built mechanism a “language acquisition device” which supports children’s language learning (McDevitt et al. , 2019). However, it is diagnosed that a child with SLI might be able to talk later than those who are also at a similar developmental stage (NIDCD, 2019). Possibly, to some extent, there is a disorder or delay in this child’s language acquisition device. Therefore, it is critical to identify children with language difficulties within the classroom settings and have appropriate teaching along with pedagogical approaches to support and facilitate their needs. SLI can be determined via many common symptoms that occur in early childhood such as having difficulties in adopting new words, putting words in order to make a complete sentence or conversation, and making frequent grammatical errors, as described in the NIDCD’s fact sheet (2019). Consequently, if no appropriate and opportune treatment is presented, this impairment might become a lifelong condition for a child. For example, it might persistently influence children’s adulthood by restricting their ability to construct complex sentences or disordering their literacy skills in later educational contexts (NIDCD, 2019).
Considering the disadvantaged consequences, it is widely discerned that children with language disorders might undergo numerous long-term outcomes including social, behavioral, and emotional challenges in their daily functioning (Schoon, Parsons, Rush, & Law, 2010). Among these difficulties, emotional development plays an essential part in children’s general development since it encompasses how children understand, express, and adjust their emotions (Margetts & Woolfolk, 2018). Most importantly, language development connects intensely to emotional development in early childhood (Durkin & Conti-Ramsden, 2010). Indeed, children with SLI are reported to be at greater risk of emotional difficulties so their depression and anxiety levels might be higher than those who typically develop (Conti-Ramsden & Botting, 2008). For instance, typical peers might have more curiosity and want to explore deeper about emotions than children having language difficulties. This example can be better explained through Spackman’s and other researchers’ practical methods (2006) in exploring reactions of children with SLI and typically developing children. Through the depiction of different emotions including happiness, sadness, excitement, or anger from a specific character, they came to deduce that children with SLI reflected and identified emotions not as accurate as other typical peers (Spackman et al. , 2006). Another equally concerning outcome is the deficiency in literacy skills found in children with SLI throughout their academic education. It is well acknowledged in empirical studies that children having difficulties with language might experience barriers to literacy which covers reading, word coding, and also writing skills (Botting, Simkin & Conti-Ramsden, 2006; Lindsay & Strand, 2016). In particular, based on the study of literacy development in children, encountering preschool language impairments might lead children to be at higher risk of dyslexia which is defined as a low ability in reading comprehension (Hulme, et al. , 2015). Furthermore, language disorders result in a low level of self-concept which means one’s lack of awareness and beliefs about self like not believing how well they can grasp knowledge at school. There is evidence that the academic self-concept of children with language difficulties is less developed than typically developing children (Lindsay et al. , 2008). According to Margetts and Hoy (2018), self-concept in academic areas such as verbal, mathematics, problem-solving, art or computers, contributes tremendously to children’s actions and motivations in learning. Thus, once children with language difficulties lack their self-concept, it might reduce their productivity, effectiveness and motivation in academic achievement.
Recognizing the vulnerability of children with SLI, practical and clinical interventions are urgently necessary. Since parents are the ones who have the most intimate relationship with their children, parental roles and participation in improving children’s language skills are vital. As reported by Colmar (2014) followed by the original works of Hoff and Tian (2005), frequent natural conversations initiated by parents using child-directed speech (CDS) provide vast communicative opportunities for children with language difficulties to practice language learning. This supportive approach can be implemented in different naturalistic contexts like play activities where children can contribute initiatives and parents can actively respond to these opinions to boost up communicative interaction. It is worth emphasizing that through book-reading activities, parents can likewise promote children’s language skills by asking them open-ended questions in correlation to the shared stories. Books are approved to contain potential debatable themes, diversified and new vocabulary which are advantageous to construct parental-child conversations (Barachetti & Lavelli, 2011; Colmar, 2014). Hence, children displaying language disorders can engage in language learning and strengthen their reading skills for later education through book-reading activities.
Besides that, Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory can be applied to get children to use language to interact with not only their parents but also other people. Many sociocultural theorists explain sociocultural theory by observing children’s language acquisition via the process of letting them socialize and interact within social contexts (McDevitt et al. , 2019). This progression is understood as language socialization which comprises both direct and indirect communication ways that children are taught to be competent in the language and verbal behaviors in their culture (McDevitt et al. , 2019). Therefore, creating social-communicative contexts combined with the regularity of natural communicative interaction initiated by parents can gain children’s proactive communication and be progressively optimal for children’s language advancement. In many theorists’ perspectives, children’s learning might be fostered through the frequency of language use and nonverbal communications (Bavin, 2009; Chater & Manning, 2006; Werker & Curtin, 2005; McDevitt et al. , 2019). In fact, in pre-school ages, children’s brains are supposed to be developing for skill acquisition to support their academic learning later on (Barrett & Hammond, 2008). Thereby, another efficient method to involve children with poor language skills in socio-emotional functioning is having early systematic interventions. Providing high-quality emotional support in learning contexts, for example, can enhance children’s language development and early reading comprehension. As reinforced by the studies of McGinty and Justice (2009) when they involved mothers and children with SLI in book-reading activities (cited in Biancone et al. , 2014). The result anticipated to be successful in awakening children’s emergent literacy skills via the application of maternal emotional and instructional support into the activities (Biancone et al. , 2014). As a result, an emotionally supportive environment plays a significant dimension in extending children’s social interactions.
In conclusion, language skills are remarkably crucial for children since they supplement other developments in children such as cognition, emotion, or social adaptation. Although children who exhibited a delay in language skills suffer several disadvantages in emotional expressions, the ability to either read or write, and self-concept, various supportive recommendations are emphasized to accelerate their special needs. Importantly, parental roles are highly suggested to be critical in accommodating and equipping children with language skills for lifelong development. Additionally, early interventions like emotional and instructional supports are likely to assist children with SLI in reinforcing their literacy skills and social functioning. The emphasis of this essay also consists of the importance of Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory in children’s language acquisition which explains how children absorb language in social contexts. If early resolutions are implemented, it can reduce unexpected risks that might be barriers to children’s transition to adulthood. The results of these supportive recommendations are supported with evidence to be effective in acceleration to children’s language skills as mentioned above.
In the earliest years of life,
language
acquisition
is considered
one of the most remarkable milestones in
children’s
development
and can be a basis for psychological and cognitive
development
(
Varanda
et al
, 2019).
Therefore
,
children
who
suffer from
language
disorders
might
experience a range of
other
successive developmental issues, which
negatively
affect their transition to adulthood. Unfortunately,
language
difficulties
were reported
to be amongst the most common developmental issues in
early
childhood which
were calculated
to be
between 7 to 17
percent in prevalence (Law, Boyle, Harris,
Harkness
, &
Nye
, 1999;
Reilly
et al.
,
2010; Cronin et al.
,
2016).
Generally
,
children
who
have
difficulties
in verbal
communication
, are kindergarten
children
around 5 to 6 years of
age
(
Beitchman
et al.
,
1986;
Tomblin
et al.
,
1997;
Norbury
et al.
,
2016; Lindsay & Strand, 2016). In the following essay, the insightful understanding of developmental
language
disorders along with the recommended treatment and therapy will
be discussed
.
According to the National Institute on Deafness and
other
Communication
Disorders (
NIDCD
) (2019),
children
who
do not struggle with hearing or intellectual disabilities
but
whose developmental
language
skills
are delayed
due to their
communication
disorders can
be identified
as Specific
Language
Impairment (SLI).
McDevitt
and
other
researchers (2019) have asserted that infants
normally
start
forming and saying their
first
few simple and recognizable words at about 12 months of
age
.
Furthermore
,
many
nativists
believed that there is an innate
language
ability
in the
early
ages
which
actively
demonstrates that during a
child’s
early
experience, he can
naturally
acquire the
language
despite no specific instruction is
given
(
McDevitt
et al.
,
2019). Chomsky (1959) called this
biologically
in-built mechanism a
“language
acquisition
device” which
supports
children’s
language
learning
(
McDevitt
et al.
,
2019).
However
, it
is diagnosed
that a
child
with SLI
might
be able to talk
later
than those
who
are
also
at a similar developmental stage (
NIDCD
, 2019).
Possibly
, to
some
extent, there is a disorder or delay in this
child’s
language
acquisition
device.
Therefore
, it is critical to identify
children
with
language
difficulties
within the classroom settings and have appropriate teaching along with pedagogical approaches to
support
and facilitate their needs. SLI can
be determined
via
many
common symptoms that occur in
early
childhood such as
having
difficulties
in adopting new words, putting words in order to
make
a complete sentence or conversation, and making frequent grammatical errors, as
described
in the
NIDCD
’s fact sheet (2019).
Consequently
, if no appropriate and opportune treatment
is presented
, this impairment
might
become a lifelong condition for a
child
.
For example
, it
might
persistently
influence
children’s
adulthood by restricting their
ability
to construct complex sentences or disordering their
literacy
skills
in
later
educational
contexts
(
NIDCD
, 2019).
Considering the disadvantaged consequences, it is
widely
discerned that
children
with
language
disorders
might
undergo numerous long-term outcomes including
social
, behavioral, and
emotional
challenges in their daily functioning (
Schoon
, Parsons, Rush, & Law, 2010).
Among
these
difficulties
,
emotional
development
plays an essential part in
children’s
general
development
since it encompasses how
children
understand, express, and adjust their
emotions
(
Margetts
&
Woolfolk
, 2018). Most
importantly
,
language
development
connects
intensely
to
emotional
development
in
early
childhood (
Durkin
&
Conti-Ramsden
, 2010).
Indeed
,
children
with SLI
are reported
to be at greater
risk
of
emotional
difficulties
so
their depression and anxiety levels
might
be higher than those
who
typically
develop (
Conti-Ramsden
&
Botting
, 2008).
For instance
, typical peers
might
have more curiosity and want to explore deeper about
emotions
than
children
having
language
difficulties
. This example can be better
explained
through
Spackman
’s and
other
researchers’ practical methods (2006) in exploring reactions of
children
with SLI and
typically
developing
children
.
Through
the depiction of
different
emotions
including happiness, sadness, excitement, or anger from a specific character, they came to deduce that
children
with SLI reflected and identified
emotions
not as accurate as
other
typical peers (
Spackman
et al.
,
2006). Another
equally
concerning outcome is the deficiency in
literacy
skills
found in
children
with SLI throughout their
academic
education. It is well acknowledged in empirical studies that
children
having
difficulties
with
language
might
experience barriers to
literacy
which covers
reading
,
word
coding, and
also
writing
skills
(
Botting
,
Simkin
&
Conti-Ramsden
, 2006; Lindsay & Strand, 2016).
In particular
, based on the study of
literacy
development
in
children
, encountering preschool
language
impairments
might
lead
children
to be at higher
risk
of dyslexia which
is defined
as a low
ability
in
reading
comprehension (
Hulme
, et al.
,
2015).
Furthermore
,
language
disorders
result
in a low level of self-concept which means one’s lack of awareness and beliefs about self like not believing how well they can grasp knowledge at school. There is evidence that the
academic
self-concept of
children
with
language
difficulties
is less developed than
typically
developing
children
(Lindsay et al.
,
2008). According to
Margetts
and
Hoy
(2018), self-concept in
academic
areas such as verbal, mathematics, problem-solving, art or computers, contributes
tremendously
to
children’s
actions and motivations in
learning
.
Thus
, once
children
with
language
difficulties
lack their self-concept, it
might
reduce
their productivity, effectiveness and motivation in
academic
achievement.
Recognizing the vulnerability of
children
with SLI, practical and clinical interventions are
urgently
necessary. Since
parents
are the
ones
who
have the most intimate relationship with their
children
, parental roles and participation in improving
children’s
language
skills
are vital. As reported by
Colmar
(2014) followed by the original works of Hoff and
Tian
(2005), frequent natural conversations initiated by
parents
using child-directed speech (CDS) provide vast communicative opportunities for
children
with
language
difficulties
to practice
language
learning
. This supportive approach can
be implemented
in
different
naturalistic
contexts
like play
activities
where
children
can contribute initiatives and
parents
can
actively
respond to these opinions to boost up communicative interaction. It is worth emphasizing that
through
book-reading
activities
,
parents
can
likewise
promote
children’s
language
skills
by asking them open-ended questions in correlation to the shared stories. Books
are approved
to contain potential debatable themes, diversified and new vocabulary which are advantageous to construct parental-child conversations (
Barachetti
&
Lavelli
, 2011;
Colmar
, 2014).
Hence
,
children
displaying
language
disorders can engage in
language
learning
and strengthen their
reading
skills
for
later
education
through
book-reading activities.
Besides
that,
Vygotsky
’s sociocultural theory can
be applied
to
get
children
to
use
language
to interact with not
only
their
parents
but
also
other
people
.
Many
sociocultural theorists
explain
sociocultural theory by observing
children’s
language
acquisition
via the process of letting them socialize and interact within
social
contexts
(
McDevitt
et al.
,
2019). This progression
is understood
as
language
socialization which comprises both direct and indirect
communication
ways that
children
are taught
to be competent in the
language
and verbal behaviors in their culture (
McDevitt
et al.
,
2019).
Therefore
, creating social-communicative
contexts
combined with the regularity of natural communicative interaction initiated by
parents
can gain
children’s
proactive
communication
and be
progressively
optimal for
children’s
language
advancement. In
many
theorists’ perspectives,
children’s
learning
might
be fostered
through
the frequency of
language
use
and nonverbal communications (
Bavin
, 2009;
Chater
& Manning, 2006;
Werker
&
Curtin
, 2005;
McDevitt
et al.
,
2019). In fact, in
pre-school
ages
,
children’s
brains
are supposed
to be developing for
skill
acquisition
to
support
their
academic
learning
later
on (Barrett & Hammond, 2008). Thereby, another efficient method to involve
children
with poor
language
skills
in
socio-emotional
functioning is
having
early
systematic interventions. Providing high-quality
emotional
support
in
learning
contexts
,
for example
, can enhance
children’s
language
development
and
early
reading
comprehension. As reinforced by the studies of
McGinty
and Justice (2009) when they involved mothers and
children
with SLI in book-reading
activities
(cited in
Biancone
et al.
,
2014). The
result
anticipated to be successful in awakening
children’s
emergent
literacy
skills
via the application of maternal
emotional
and instructional
support
into the
activities
(
Biancone
et al.
,
2014). As a
result
, an
emotionally
supportive environment plays a significant dimension in extending
children’s
social
interactions.
In conclusion
,
language
skills
are
remarkably
crucial for
children
since they supplement
other
developments
in
children
such as cognition,
emotion
, or
social
adaptation. Although
children
who
exhibited a delay in
language
skills
suffer several disadvantages in
emotional
expressions, the
ability
to either read or write, and self-concept, various supportive recommendations
are emphasized
to accelerate their special needs.
Importantly
, parental roles are
highly
suggested to be
critical in accommodating and equipping
children
with
language
skills
for lifelong
development
.
Additionally
,
early
interventions like
emotional
and instructional
supports
are likely to assist
children
with SLI in reinforcing their
literacy
skills
and
social
functioning. The emphasis of this essay
also
consists of the importance of
Vygotsky
’s sociocultural theory in
children’s
language
acquisition
which
explains
how
children
absorb
language
in
social
contexts
. If
early
resolutions
are implemented
, it can
reduce
unexpected
risks
that
might
be barriers to
children’s
transition to adulthood. The
results
of these supportive recommendations
are supported
with evidence to be effective in acceleration to
children’s
language
skills
as mentioned above.