The Creators of Grammar
No student of a foreign language needs to be told that grammar is complex. By changing word sequences and by adding a range of auxiliary verbs and suffixes, we are able to communicate tiny variations in meaning. We can turn a statement into a question, state whether an action has taken place or is soon to take place, and perform many other word tricks to convey subtle differences in meaning. Nor is this complexity inherent to the English language. All languages, even those of so-called 'primitive' tribes have clever grammatical components. The Cherokee pronoun system, for example, can distinguish between 'you and I', 'several other people and I' and 'you, another person and I'. In English, all these meanings are summed up in the one, crude pronoun 'we'. Grammar is universal and plays a part in every language, no matter how widespread it is. So the question which has baffled many linguists is - who created grammar?
At first, it would appear that this question is impossible to answer. To find out how grammar is created, someone needs to be present at the time of a language's creation, documenting its emergence. Many historical linguists are able to trace modern complex languages back to earlier languages, but in order to answer the question of how complex languages are actually formed, the researcher needs to observe how languages are started from scratch. Amazingly, however, this is possible.
Some of the most recent languages evolved due to the Atlantic slave trade. At that time, slaves from a number of different ethnicities were forced to work together under colonizer's rule. Since they had no opportunity to learn each other's languages, they developed a make-shift language called a pidgin. Pidgins are strings of words copied from the language of the landowner. They have little in the way of grammar, and in many cases it is difficult for a listener to deduce when an event happened, and who did what to whom. [A] Speakers need to use circumlocution in order to make their meaning understood. [B] Interestingly, however, all it takes for a pidgin to become a complex language is for a group of children to be exposed to it at the time when they learn their mother tongue. [C] Slave children did not simply copy the strings of words uttered by their elders, they adapted their words to create a new, expressive language. [D] Complex grammar systems which emerge from pidgins are termed creoles, and they are invented by children.
Further evidence of this can be seen in studying sign languages for the deaf. Sign languages are not simply a series of gestures; they utilise the same grammatical machinery that is found in spoken languages. Moreover, there are many different languages used worldwide. The creation of one such language was documented quite recently in Nicaragua. Previously, all deaf people were isolated from each other, but in 1979 a new government introduced schools for the deaf. Although children were taught speech and lip reading in the classroom, in the playgrounds they began to invent their own sign system, using the gestures that they used at home. It was basically a pidgin. Each child used the signs differently, and there was no consistent grammar. However, children who joined the school later, when this inventive sign system was already around, developed a quite different sign language. Although it was based on the signs of the older children, the younger children's language was more fluid and compact, and it utilised a large range of grammatical devices to clarify meaning. What is more, all the children used the signs in the same way. A new creole was born.
Some linguists believe that many of the world's most established languages were creoles at first. The English past tense –ed ending may have evolved from the verb 'do'. 'It ended' may once have been 'It end-did'. Therefore it would appear that even the most widespread languages were partly created by children. Children appear to have innate grammatical machinery in their brains, which springs to life when they are first trying to make sense of the world around them. Their minds can serve to create logical, complex structures, even when there is no grammar present for them to copy.
The Creators of Grammar
No student of a foreign
language
needs
to be
told
that
grammar
is
complex
. By changing
word
sequences and by adding a range of auxiliary verbs and suffixes, we are able to communicate tiny variations in
meaning
. We can turn a statement into a
question
, state whether an action has taken place or is
soon
to take place, and perform
many
other
word
tricks to convey subtle differences in
meaning
. Nor is this complexity inherent to the English
language
. All
languages
, even those of
so
-called 'primitive' tribes have clever
grammatical
components. The Cherokee pronoun
system
,
for example
, can distinguish between 'you and I', 'several other
people
and I' and 'you, another person and I'. In English, all these
meanings
are summed
up in the one, crude pronoun 'we'.
Grammar
is universal and plays a part in every
language
, no matter how widespread it is.
So
the
question
which has baffled
many
linguists is
-
who created grammar?
At
first
, it would appear that this
question
is impossible to answer. To find out how
grammar
is created
, someone
needs
to be present at the time of a language's creation, documenting its emergence.
Many
historical linguists are able to trace modern
complex
languages
back to earlier
languages
,
but
in order to answer the
question
of how
complex
languages
are actually formed, the researcher
needs
to observe how
languages
are
started
from scratch.
Amazingly
,
however
, this is possible.
Some
of the most recent
languages
evolved due to the Atlantic slave trade. At that time, slaves from a number of
different
ethnicities
were forced
to work together under colonizer's
rule
. Since they had no opportunity to learn each other's
languages
, they developed a
make
-shift
language
called a
pidgin
.
Pidgins
are strings of words copied from the
language
of the landowner. They have
little
in the way of
grammar
, and in
many
cases it is difficult for a listener to deduce when an
event
happened, and who did what to whom. [A] Speakers
need
to
use
circumlocution in order to
make
their
meaning
understood. [B]
Interestingly
,
however
, all it takes for a
pidgin
to become a
complex
language
is for a group of
children
to
be exposed
to it at the time when they learn their mother tongue. [C] Slave
children
did not
simply
copy the strings of words uttered by their elders, they adapted their words to create a new, expressive
language
. [D]
Complex
grammar
systems
which emerge from
pidgins
are termed
creoles, and they
are invented
by children.
Further
evidence of this can be
seen
in studying
sign
languages
for the deaf.
Sign
languages
are not
simply
a series of gestures; they
utilise
the same
grammatical
machinery that
is found
in spoken
languages
.
Moreover
, there are
many
different
languages
used
worldwide. The creation of one such
language
was documented
quite recently in Nicaragua. Previously, all deaf
people
were isolated
from each other,
but
in 1979 a new
government
introduced schools for the deaf. Although
children
were taught
speech and
lip reading
in the classroom, in the playgrounds they began to invent their
own
sign
system
, using the gestures that they
used
at home. It was
basically
a
pidgin
. Each child
used
the
signs
differently
, and there was no consistent
grammar
.
However
,
children
who
joined
the school later, when this inventive
sign
system
was already around, developed a quite
different
sign
language
. Although it
was based
on the
signs
of the older
children
, the younger children's
language
was more fluid and compact, and it
utilised
a large range of
grammatical
devices to clarify
meaning
.
What is more
, all the
children
used
the
signs
in the same way
. A new creole
was born
.
Some
linguists believe that
many
of the world's most established
languages
were creoles at
first
. The English past tense –ed ending may have evolved from the verb 'do'. 'It ended' may once have been 'It
end
-did'.
Therefore
it would appear that even the most widespread
languages
were partly created by
children
.
Children
appear to have innate
grammatical
machinery in their brains, which springs to life when they are
first
trying to
make
sense of the world around them. Their minds can serve to create logical,
complex
structures, even when there is no
grammar
present for them to copy.